The Diet Doctor policy for grading scientific evidence

As we base our guides on scientific evidence, it is important to have a clear policy for how to grade the strength of different kinds of evidence. Our policy is in many ways similar to other documents of its kind.
We focus on how scientific evidence applies to human clinical outcomes, and we define the levels of evidence as follows.

  1. Systematic reviews or meta-analyses of multiple, high-quality randomized controlled trials:
    Strong evidence.

    Systematic reviews or meta-analyses of observational studies with a hazard ratio (HR) under 2:

    Very weak evidence.

    Systematic reviews or meta-analyses of observational studies with HR>2:Weak evidence.

    Systematic reviews or meta-analyses of observational studies with HR>5 and generally following the Bradford Hill Criteria: Moderate evidence.

  2. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs): Moderate evidence. If repeated, very clear and consistent results over multiple trials, then it may be upgraded to: Strong evidence.
    Non-randomized or uncontrolled trials: Weak evidence. Note that while non-randomized trials are weak for determining the best intervention, they can provide other unique insights.
  3. Observational/epidemiological data is upgraded in strength if the hazard ratio (HR) is over 2.
    With a HR below 2, evidence shows that the correlation is often misleading and false, and it may be more likely to confuse than to inform:
    Very weak evidence.

    With a HR > 2 in high-quality prospective cohort studies, or on occasion with a very large population sample and consistent findings across studies even with HR < 2: Weak evidence.

    Or, if an observational study assesses the prevalence of a condition, but does not attempt to correlate observations with a health outcome, that is Weak evidence.

    With a HR > 2 in lower-quality observational studies: Very weak evidence.

    Under exceptional and rare circumstances with HR consistently > 5 in several high-quality observational studies, with biological plausibility, no other obvious explanation and generally following the classic Bradford Hill criteria:

    Moderate evidence (e.g. smoking and lung cancer).

  4. Note: If a study uses an odd ratio (OR), we consider that equivalent to a hazard ratio or relative risk if the prevalence of the measured condition is low. However, if the prevalence is high and an adjusted odds ratio is reported, we will defer to the adjusted OR.

  5. Consistent clinical experience (e.g. case series) from several experienced practitioners is also considered, as long as there’s no high-quality science (i.e. randomized intervention studies) to contradict it: Weak evidence.
  6. Case reports and anecdotes are evaluated with caution and not used as evidence unless there’s a lack of higher-quality evidence available. We will not grade individual anecdotes as evidence, but may grade a collection of anecdotes or published case reports as Very weak evidence.
  7. Animal studies are not considered in fields where studies already exist on humans. If it’s the only evidence available: Very weak evidence.
  8. Mechanistic studies and cell studies are considered very weak evidence, lower than even animal studies. These studies may be experimental or may discuss well-known mechanisms but apply them to a novel explanation of a disease or intervention. Whether these mechanisms can be applied to humans in the way discussed is often unknown, even if the mechanism itself is well-established. If clinical data on the subject exist, we will usually refrain from citing mechanistic studies. If it is the only evidence available: Very weak evidence.
  9. Opinions of world-leading experts: Ungraded. Opinions are not evidence, no matter who has the opinion. To be evidence-based, everyone has to support his or her opinions and theories with believable facts, i.e. scientific evidence.


 

Overview articles

Non-systematic review papers, or comprehensive summaries, are usually a compilation of a few or dozens of individual studies, of different levels of evidence. Unless done systematically to answer a specific question (see #1 above) it’s usually hard or inappropriate to assign one specific level of evidence. These articles are instead marked like this: Overview article.

To assign a specific level of evidence we need to point to the specific studies supporting it.


 

Other ungraded articles

Some articles found in nutrition and medical journals don’t fall into one of the above categories. These are some of the other types of articles you may see referenced in Diet Doctor guides.

Review articles: These articles summarize a topic and may or may not include a balanced review of the literature. They may be heavily influenced by bias without much control over evidence quality. As a result, these articles are ungraded, like this: Review article.

Technical articles: These articles typically describe a new biomedical technique, procedure, or intervention, or the modification of an existing one. These articles are descriptive rather than investigative in nature and are therefore ungraded, like this: Technical article.


 

Success stories

As mentioned above under “case reports and anecdotes,” success stories are considered very weak evidence when it comes to determining if a particular lifestyle change is beneficial. There are several reasons for this, but perhaps most important is that these stories come from a selected population of successful people. We don’t know how many people may have attempted the same lifestyle change without being happy with the results, or only had more moderate success.

However, these stories can add value when it comes to more deeply understanding the subjective experience and feelings of a selected group. This can potentially add another level of insight and inspiration that numbers and statistics alone can’t provide.


 

Financial bias

When citing evidence from studies at high risk of financial bias (e.g. studies about a drug funded and conducted by the company selling the drug), we note any obvious conflicts of interest and include that along with the grade.

When nutrition studies are funded by companies/industries with a financial interest in the outcome, we note that bias as well.


 

Evolutionary considerations

Consistency with what is evolutionarily probable can strengthen evidence, and inconsistency can weaken evidence.

For example, humans and their ancestors have been eating natural saturated fats for millions of years.

It’s evolutionarily unlikely that eating it in amounts roughly similar to before is a main cause of a new epidemic of chronic disease.

On the other hand, refined pure sugar in large quantities is a phenomenon of the last 150 years. It’s evolutionarily possible that it could have negative health consequences, as humans don’t appear to have adapted to it.

While these examples do not prove cause and effect, they do add context to the scientific evidence.

From this perspective it can also be argued that interventions based on eating certain natural foods — or avoiding foods for a short time (e.g. for one day) — are things that the human body should be evolutionarily adapted to, to a large degree. These interventions may thus be significantly safer, compared to new drugs or surgical interventions.

The evolutionary lens as a way to strengthen or weaken evidence has to be used with a large dose of caution, as it’s not always apparent what environmental factors our ancestors were exposed to, and many of these factors have varied widely over time and geography.


 

Core references

 
Advances in Nutrition 2018: Limiting dependence on nonrandomized studies and improving randomized trials in human nutrition research: why and how

JAMA 2018: The challenge of reforming nutritional epidemiologic research

PLoS Medicine 2005: Why most published research findings are false

The Lancet 2019: Real-world studies no substitute for RCTs in establishing efficacy


 

More

The Diet Doctor policy for evidence-based guides

The science of low carb and keto

Guide to observational vs. experimental studies

Comments

Do you have comments on, objections to or suggested changes or additions to our guidelines? Feel free to email your suggestions to andreas@dietdoctor.com.